Human brain can hear words before they are uttered

By IANS,

Washington : Scientists have shown how the brain automatically considers many possible words and their meanings before we’ve even heard the final sound of the word.


Support TwoCircles

Previous theories have proposed that listeners can only keep pace with the rapid pace of spoken language – up to five syllables per second – by anticipating a small subset of all words known by the listener, much like Google search anticipates words and phrases as you type.

This subset consists of all words that begin with the same sounds, such as “candle”, “candy,” and “cantaloupe,” and makes the task of understanding the specific word more efficient than waiting until all the sounds of the word have been presented.

But until now, researchers had no way to know if the brain also considers the meanings of these possible words. The new findings are the first time that scientists, using an MRI scanner, have been able to actually see this split-second brain activity.

The study was a team effort among former Rochester graduate student Kathleen Pirog Revill, now a postdoctoral researcher at Georgia Tech, and three faculty members in the department of brain and cognitive sciences at the University of Rochester.

“We had to figure out a way to catch the brain doing something so fast that it happens literally between spoken syllables,” said Michael Tanenhaus, a professor.

“The best tool we have for brain imaging of this sort is functional MRI, but an fMRI takes a few seconds to capture an image, so people thought it just couldn’t be done,” he said.

But it could be done. It just took inventing a new language to do it.

With William R. Kenan Richard Aslin and Daphne Bavelier, Revill focused on a tiny part of the brain called “V5,” activated when a person sees motion.

The idea was to teach undergraduates a set of invented words, some of which meant “movement,” and then to watch and see if the V5 area became activated when the subject heard words that sounded similar to the ones that meant “movement.”

But the team couldn’t use English words because a word as simple as “kick” has so many nuances of meaning. The team had to create a set of words that had similar beginning syllables, but with different ending syllables and distinct meanings – one of which meant motion of the sort that would activate the V5 area.

The team created a computer programme that showed irregular shapes and gave the shapes specific names, like “goki.” They also created new verb words. Some, like “biduko” meant “the shape will move across the screen,” whereas some, like “biduka,” meant the shape would just change colour.

After a number of students learned the new words well enough, the team tested them as they lay in an fMRI scanner. The students would see one of the shapes on a monitor and hear “biduko,” or “biduka.”

Though only one of the words actually meant “motion,” the V5 area of the brain still activated for both, although less so for the colour word than for the motion word.

The presence of some activation to the colour word shows that the brain, for a split-second, considered the motion meaning of both possible words before it heard the final, discriminating syllable – ka rather than ko.

“Frankly, we’re amazed we could detect something so subtle,” said Aslin. “But it just makes sense that your brain would do it this way. Why wait until the end of the word to try to figure out what its meaning is?

SUPPORT TWOCIRCLES HELP SUPPORT INDEPENDENT AND NON-PROFIT MEDIA. DONATE HERE